globalchange  > 气候变化事实与影响
DOI: doi:10.1038/nclimate2814
论文题名:
Co-benefits of addressing climate change can motivate action around the world
作者: Paul ; G. Bain
刊名: Nature Climate Change
ISSN: 1758-742X
EISSN: 1758-6862
出版年: 2015-09-28
卷: Volume:6, 页码:Pages:154;157 (2016)
语种: 英语
英文关键词: Psychology ; Climate-change mitigation
英文摘要:

Personal and political action on climate change is traditionally thought to be motivated by people accepting its reality and importance. However, convincing the public that climate change is real faces powerful ideological obstacles1, 2, 3, 4, and climate change is slipping in public importance in many countries5, 6. Here we investigate a different approach, identifying whether potential co-benefits of addressing climate change7 could motivate pro-environmental behaviour around the world for both those convinced and unconvinced that climate change is real. We describe an integrated framework for assessing beliefs about co-benefits8, distinguishing social conditions (for example, economic development, reduced pollution or disease) and community character (for example, benevolence, competence). Data from all inhabited continents (24 countries; 6,196 participants) showed that two co-benefit types, Development (economic and scientific advancement) and Benevolence (a more moral and caring community), motivated public, private and financial actions to address climate change to a similar degree as believing climate change is important. Critically, relationships were similar for both convinced and unconvinced participants, showing that co-benefits can motivate action across ideological divides. These relationships were also independent of perceived climate change importance, and could not be explained by political ideology, age, or gender. Communicating co-benefits could motivate action on climate change where traditional approaches have stalled.

Those trying to motivate widespread action on climate change face two hurdles. The first is to convince enough people that climate change is real and important. The second is to move people from accepting its reality and importance to acting, both in pressuring their governments and in their personal lives. A single strategy has typically been used to overcome both hurdles: present the science and consequences of climate change in more compelling ways9.

This intuitive strategy was initially successful, but in many places progress has stalled or even reversed. Communicating climate science is now failing to persuade those who remain unconvinced climate change is real (‘unconvinced, or climate sceptics)10, and the public priority of climate change is declining in many countries5, 6. These issues are strongly linked to political ideology1, 2, 3, 4, giving cause for pessimism—if people need to shift their basic political ideologies to act on climate change, the prospect for further progress is bleak.

New approaches are emerging that could sidestep these hurdles. One promising approach has been to highlight the co-benefits for society from acting on climate change7, referring to community benefits resulting from mitigation behaviours. As examples, mitigation efforts can reduce pollution11, 12, support economic development through green industries13, 14, or benefit population health by reducing disease or promoting healthier lifestyles (for example, cycling/walking instead of driving)12, 15, 16. A less obvious co-benefit involves community functioning, where climate change action can contribute to a more benevolent (caring and moral) community8, 17.

One advantage of co-benefits is that they can appeal to people unconvinced or unconcerned about climate change, as they do not depend on believing climate change is real or important. However, two challenges remain for establishing their effectiveness in motivating public action. First, researchers have focused on some co-benefits, such as reduced pollution or economic development, without an integrated approach to understand how co-benefits are related and comparing their importance for motivating public action. Second, climate change requires a global solution, but most co-benefits research has been conducted in Western countries (for example, USA (ref. 16)). It is therefore unclear whether some co-benefits are more influential in different countries, similar to the variation observed in climate change risk perceptions across countries18.

Our research addresses these challenges by providing an integrated framework for examining co-benefits, and by collecting data from around the world. By showing how perceptions of co-benefits are related to peoples motivations to act on climate change around the world, the findings could help researchers, policymakers and communicators develop effective local and global strategies for using co-benefits to motivate action.

Data were obtained from 24 countries spanning all inhabited continents and with diverse carbon emission levels (see Supplementary Table 1). University student samples were selected to facilitate comparisons, as students typically occupy similar socio-economic positions across countries. We also obtained community samples in ten countries to establish the generalizability and robustness of findings.

Research participants first indicated their beliefs about the reality and importance of climate change. Those who believed climate change is real (‘convinced) considered what their nation would be like in the future if action had successfully mitigated climate change. Those unconvinced that climate change is real, for whom successful mitigation is not applicable, considered what their nation would be like in the future if people had taken action aimed at mitigating climate change.

Participants then considered the potential co-benefits for their society in these scenarios. To develop an integrated framework, we noted that many co-benefits, such as economic development, new technologies, and improvements in disease or poverty, are captured in a model of peoples beliefs about the future of society that has been validated across a wide range of social issues, including climate change8, 17. We used this ‘collective futures model and added two mitigation co-benefits for this research: pollution, and green space (extent of parks and reserves).

The collective futures model has four dimensions of co-benefits. Two dimensions address the social ‘conditions in which people live: Development (for example, economic development, scientific progress) and Dysfunction (for example, pollution, disease). Two further dimensions address the ‘character of people in society: Benevolence (whether people are caring and moral) and Competence (whether people are skilled and capable), reflecting the fundamental dimensions used to understand groups19, 20. Participants indicated whether these co-benefits would improve or worsen in their society (for example, if there were to be greater/lesser economic development, or if people would become more/less moral). The four dimensions formed reliable scales, as in previous research8, 17, indicating that people see close relationships between some co-benefits (for example, pollution and disease were components of a broader Dysfunction dimension), with lower reliabilities for unconvinced samples in a few countries (see Supplementary Section 1).

We examined how these co-benefit dimensions were related to three measures of motivations to act on climate change21. The first assessed public and political actions (citizenship), such as voting for pro-environmental politicians and contributing time/money to pro-environmental groups. The second involved personal domestic actions, such as conserving energy and green consumerism. The third measured financial behaviour (donation), where participants were entered into a prize draw (150 US dollars in local currency), and committed an amount for the researchers to donate to a pro-environmental organization if they won.

Correlations between these variables were computed in each country, and meta-analysis22 was used to identify how each co-benefit dimension was related to motivations to act. Meta-analysis computes the average correlation across all samples (effect size) weighted by sample size, with a 95% confidence interval indicating the likely range of this correlation. Meta-analysis also identifies whether the magnitude of the correlations varies substantially across the samples (Q-statistic).

We first established the strength of relationships between co-benefits and motivations to act, including climate change importance as a benchmark. To provide the toughest test of the additional value of co-benefits, we focused first on ‘convinced participants, who were expected to show strong effects for climate change importance. Figure 1 shows that believing climate change is important had the strongest effect size across all action measures for student samples (n = 4,049). However, this effect varied significantly across countries. Critically, two co-benefits had effects of a comparable size to climate change importance. Development showed the strongest effect sizes for citizenship and personal actions and a weaker effect for donations, with effect sizes also varying across countries. Effect sizes for Benevolence were also relatively strong but were less variable across countries.

Figure 1: Meta-analyses showing average effect sizes (with 95% confidence intervals) and tests for cross-country variability (Q) for climate change importance and co-benefit dimensions with motivations to act on climate change for ‘convinced participants (n = 4,049) across 24 countries.
Meta-analyses showing average effect sizes (with 95% confidence intervals) and tests for cross-country variability (Q) for climate change importance and co-benefit dimensions with motivations to act on climate change for /`convinced[rsquor] participants (n = 4,049) across 24 countries.

p < 0.05, p < 0.01, p < 0.005. Citizenship refers to public/political behaviours, Personal to domestic behaviours, and Donation to financial behaviour. The bars show the average correlation effect across countries, with the 95% confidence interval for this average effect. The Q-statistic evaluates cross-country variability in effect sizes. Development and Benevolence co-benefits showed comparable effect sizes to climate change importance across the behavioural measures. Q-statistics show that climate change importance and Development/Dysfunction co-benefits varied in their effects across countries, whereas Character co-benefits showed more consistent effects across countries.

The sections below describe the samples, data collection procedures, measures, and basic analytical approach. Additional information is provided in Supplementary Information.

Samples and data collection.

Data were collected from 24 countries (24 student samples and 10 community samples) in the period of June 2013 to July 2014. Countries were targeted to span geographic regions and a wide range of climate change contributions based on the Environmental Performance Index24, which reflects national CO2 emissions (both industrial and residential) and traditional/renewable energy production. Countries included 11 high carbon emitters (USA, Netherlands, Russia, Poland, Germany, Australia, China, Japan, South Korea, Israel, South Africa), 9 medium emitters (United Kingdom, France, Spain, Sweden, Norway, Venezuela, Mexico, Chile, New Zealand), and 4 low emitters (Brazil, Switzerland, Iceland, Ghana). Contributors from additional countries were involved in the research (especially in low-emitter countries), but were not able to provide a viable sample within the data collection time frame. Sample details are shown in Supplementary Table 1.

Contributors in each country were instructed to obtain a student sample of citizens from their country (target N = 200), aiming for an even gender split and a diversity of study disciplines. Contributors who agreed to provide community samples were instructed to obtain a non-student citizen sample (target N = 200). Community samples were typically sourced through commercial market research companies who specialize in recruiting across a country population, but some were convenience samples based on local recruitment strategies (for example, Poland).

Participants completed a survey developed with feedback from country contributors for applicability and relevance. Surveys were completed either online (17 countries) or on paper where local contributors viewed online administration as less practical (7 countries: Ghana, Japan, Mexico, Poland, South Korea, Spain and Venezuela). The paper version of the survey adhered strictly to a template developed by the project coordinators to ensure consistency and to match the online surveys.

Surveys were completed in the major local language, except in Switzerland, Ghana and South Africa, where multiple major languages are spoken. Swiss participants could complete the survey in German or French. The common language of student instruction was used in Ghana (English) and South Africa (English or Afrikaans). Translations were obtained using translation-back-translation by competent bilingual speakers, or using parallel translation where multiple bilingual speakers independently translated the survey. In both approaches, discussion of discrepancies between the translators and the project coordinators continued until an acceptable translation was agreed on.

Measures.

The research project was designed to address several research questions in addition to those reported in the article. Below we describe the measures used in the article, and describe additional measures in Supplementary Section 1. Reliability indices (Cronbachs alphas) for multi-item scales and descriptive statistics for all measures are shown in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3, respectively.

Climate change importance. Participants first rated the item measuring perceived climate change importance, which was embedded among other items: “Addressing climate change is one of the most important issues facing society today (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree).

Climate change beliefs. Participants then completed a screening item asking them to choose from the following three options used in previous research17: “(1) I believe climate change is occurring, and human activities are having significant effects on climate change; (2) I believe climate change is occurring, but human activities are not having significant effects on climate change; or (3) I do not believe climate change is occurring. Participants who selected (1) were categorized as believing in anthropogenic climate change (‘convinced), and those who chose (2) or (3) were categorized as unconvinced or sceptical about the reality of anthropogenic climate change (‘unconvinced).

Co-benefits scenarios. To give participants a context for thinking about co-benefits of climate change action, we asked them to think about what their society would be like in the future. Specifically, convinced participants were instructed to think about their country in 2050 where people have taken action that has prevented significant climate change. Unconvinced participants were instructed to think about their country in 2050 where people have taken action aimed at preventing significant climate change. The reason for using separate scenarios for convinced and unconvinced participants arose from pilot testing: the scenario where climate change was prevented (used for convinced participants) was deemed unsuitable for those unconvinced, and responses from a substantial number of convinced participants indicated they interpreted the more general scenario (used for unconvinced participants here) as indicating that action was not successful. Participants were then instructed to imagine what their country would be like in this scenario, and then proceeded to answer the co-benefit measures.

Co-benefits measures. For the scenario participants were instructed to imagine, they then rated their country in 2050 on scales from the validated collective futures model17, in which conditions, character and societal values are distinguished.

Conditions. Participants rated the extent to which the following aspects of their country would become worse or improve compared to today (−5 = much worse, 0 = same as today, +5 = much improved). The aspects of society reflected Development and Dysfunction dimensions. Development items were ‘economic development, ‘education levels, ‘volunteering, ‘scientific progress and ‘extent of community groups. Dysfunction items were ‘violent crime, ‘poverty, ‘disease, ‘pollution, ‘theft and ‘unemployment levels.

Character. Participants rated how typical a list of personal characteristics would be of people in their country compared to today (−5 = much less typical than today, 0 = same as today, +5 = much more typical than today). These characteristics reflected two dimensions. Benevolence items were ‘caring, ‘warm, ‘considerate, ‘honest, ‘sincere, ‘trustworthy, ‘unfriendly (reversed), ‘immoral (reversed), ‘insensitive (reversed) and ‘unethical (reversed). Competence items were ‘competent, ‘capable, ‘assertive, ‘lazy (reversed) and ‘unskilled (reversed).

Societal values. Ratings of how values would change in society were also made, using 12 values selected from the Schwartz Value Survey31 to reflect four quadrants (three values per quadrant). However, scales created to reflect each quadrant showed low reliabilities across many countries and were excluded from analyses.

Motivations to act on climate change. The three pro-environmental measures were presented in the context of addressing climate change.

Citizenship. Environmental citizenship intentions items were taken from an existing measure32, and adapted and extended based on feedback from contributors in the different countries. The final 12-item scale focuses on behaviours aimed at bringing about public and political action. Items added included exerting influence within a persons social network (friendship networks and social media). The scale header and items were: “Many individuals and groups interested in protecting the environment believe addressing climate change is a key concern. With this in mind, how likely are you to engage in the following activities in the next 12 months? (If it is not possible for you to perform an activity, please choose ‘not applicable.) (1 = not at all likely, 5 = very likely, na = not applicable.) (1) Sign a petition in support of protecting the environment. (2) Join or renew membership of an environmental group. (3) Join public demonstrations or protests supporting environmental protection. (4) Write a letter or call your member of Parliament or another government official to support environmental protection. (5) Give money to an environmental group. (6) Read a newsletter, magazine or other publication written by an environmental group. (7) If a local, state or Federal election was called, vote for a candidate at least in part because he or she was in favour of strong environmental protection. (8) Write to a newspaper in support of protecting the environment. (9) Boycott companies that are not environmentally friendly. (10) Volunteer to help an environmental group or event. (11) Post pro-environmental messages or links on social media (for example, Facebook, Twitter). (12) Speak in favour of pro-environmental policies in conversations with your friends or family. As some behaviours were less applicable to some people and in some countries, scale scores were created where participants provided answers (excluding ‘not applicable) for at least six items. The ‘not applicable choice was selected less than a third of the time for every item in every country, indicating that overall participants believed these behaviours were possible in their country. To create scores for each individual, missing and ‘not applicable items were excluded, and the scale score was computed by averaging over the remaining items.

Personal. Personal sphere behavioural intentions differ from citizenship in focusing on household domestic behaviours. This scale was drawn from several sourcesURL:

http://www.nature.com/nclimate/journal/v6/n2/full/nclimate2814.html
Citation statistics:
资源类型: 期刊论文
标识符: http://119.78.100.158/handle/2HF3EXSE/4570
Appears in Collections:气候变化事实与影响
科学计划与规划
气候变化与战略

Files in This Item: Download All
File Name/ File Size Content Type Version Access License
nclimate2814.pdf(460KB)期刊论文作者接受稿开放获取View Download

Recommended Citation:
Paul ,G. Bain. Co-benefits of addressing climate change can motivate action around the world[J]. Nature Climate Change,2015-09-28,Volume:6:Pages:154;157 (2016).
Service
Recommend this item
Sava as my favorate item
Show this item's statistics
Export Endnote File
Google Scholar
Similar articles in Google Scholar
[Paul ]'s Articles
[G. Bain]'s Articles
百度学术
Similar articles in Baidu Scholar
[Paul ]'s Articles
[G. Bain]'s Articles
CSDL cross search
Similar articles in CSDL Cross Search
[Paul ]‘s Articles
[G. Bain]‘s Articles
Related Copyright Policies
Null
收藏/分享
文件名: nclimate2814.pdf
格式: Adobe PDF
此文件暂不支持浏览
所有评论 (0)
暂无评论
 

Items in IR are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.