globalchange  > 气候变化事实与影响
DOI: doi:10.1038/nclimate2756
论文题名:
Australians' views on carbon pricing before and after the 2013 federal election
作者: Stacia J. Dreyer
刊名: Nature Climate Change
ISSN: 1758-798X
EISSN: 1758-6918
出版年: 2015-08-10
卷: Volume:5, 页码:Pages:1064;1067 (2015)
语种: 英语
英文关键词: Climate change ; Carbon and energy ; Psychology ; Economics
英文摘要:

As climate policies change through the legislative process, public attitudes towards them may change as well. Therefore, it is important to assess how people accept and support controversial climate policies as the policies change over time. Policy acceptance is a positive evaluation of, or attitude towards, an existing policy1, 2, 3; policy support adds an active behavioural component1, 3. Acceptance does not necessarily lead to support. We conducted a national survey of Australian residents to investigate acceptance of, and support for, the Australian carbon pricing policy before and after the 2013 federal election, and how perceptions of the policy, economic ideology, and voting behaviour affect acceptance and support. We found acceptance and support were stable across the election period, which was surprising given that climate policy was highly contentious during the election. Policy acceptance was higher than policy support at both times and acceptance was a necessary but insufficient condition of support. We conclude that acceptance is an important process through which perceptions of the policy and economic ideology influence support. Therefore, future climate policy research needs to distinguish between acceptance and support to better understand this process, and to better measure these concepts.

The acceptability of an environmental policy usually increases between planning and implementation4, 5, 6. This pattern is also evident in controversial health policies such as smoking restrictions7, but is not well established in the climate policy literature, where researchers have measured acceptability, acceptance, and/or support for climate policies only at single time points1, 8. Increased acceptance and support may be due to increased familiarity9, 10 with the policy or the realization that prior anxieties regarding perceived negative outcomes of the policy were unfulfilled4.

The role elections play in changing policy support and acceptance is poorly understood. We examined changes in support for, and acceptance of, Australias carbon pricing policy using data collected a fortnight before and a fortnight after the 2013 federal election. The policy was a heated topic of debate during the election campaign. Regardless of familiarity processes, changes in carbon policy support and acceptance may reflect or drive changes in the government, but the literature does not lend itself to any firm hypothesis. If the newly elected government had sought to overturn an existing policy as part of its election platform, we may expect support and acceptance to diminish. If the incumbent government, which had introduced the policy, were re-elected, we may expect support and acceptance to increase. Many processes may account for such changes. People may alter their views to align with newly discovered population norms11, they may seek to resolve cognitive dissonance arising from differences between their views and the new status quo12, 13, or their views may become stronger because of greater perceived consensus for their own views11.

Policy acceptability, acceptance and support have been used interchangeably within the literature, but represent distinct constructs1, 2, 3. Disagreement also exists within the policy and renewable energy technology (RET) literature regarding the definition of these constructs. Some authors distinguish among these terms on an attitudinal/behavioural dimension1, 2, 3, 14 but, even so, do not agree which terms correspond to attitudes and which to behaviours. Acceptability has been said to correspond to attitudes and acceptance to behaviour14; conversely, acceptance to attitudes and support to behaviours1, 2, 3. Also, the terms have been distinguished temporally (that is, before (acceptability) and after (acceptance) implementation), while being unaligned with the attitudinal/behavioural dimension15. Different types of RET acceptance have also been proposed—socio-political acceptance, market acceptance, and community acceptance16.

Research measuring acceptance and support as distinct constructs shows that levels of acceptance are higher than support for the carbon policy in Australia1, fuel economy standards in the United States3, and energy infrastructure in the United Kingdom and Norway2. However, measurement concerns such as using a single item to measure the construct with different response formats1, or using the term ‘in favour of as a proxy for support in a two-item measure2, highlight the need for additional research.

Based on earlier work1, we operationalize policy acceptance as a function of attitudes towards a policy at a given time (after implementation), and policy support as a function of acceptance with an additional behavioural component (action or intent). This conceptual formulation can also be extended to RET and suggests that acceptance may be a precondition of support. (Note that acceptability is also function of attitudes, but exists before policy implementation.)

Perceived effectiveness and fairness have both been linked to the acceptability (acceptance) of a proposed (implemented) environmental policy and support of a policy: greater perceived fairness/effectiveness leads to greater acceptability, acceptance and/or support1, 3, 4, 17. Other individual characteristics are important too. For example, free-market ideology (FMI) describes the belief that markets should be allowed to operate unrestrained by government regulations; the market will resolve any problems through supply and demand dynamics18. Individuals endorsing a FMI are unlikely to accept or support climate policies1, 3 because these policies are governmental regulations seeking to minimize negative market externalities (greenhouse gas emissions).

In Australia, climate action has been heatedly debated, with both Labor and Liberal governments advancing and regressing action19, 20, 21. The Clean Energy Legislative Package (CELP, also known as the carbon pricing policy, or inaccurately but more commonly as the carbon tax) was enacted in July 2012 by the Labor-led Australian federal government. The government sought to hold about 500 large industries responsible for the emissions they release by introducing a carbon price which would, in time, function as an emissions trading scheme (ETS) tied to the European Unions ETS. The government also created a compensation package for households indirectly impacted by the carbon price. At the time of policy implementation, the Opposition coalition strongly opposed the policy, instead favouring other measures such as market incentives. However, in past years, leaders of the Liberal party (that is, Turnbull, Howard) have supported an ETS (refs 19,20,21).

In June 2013, Kevin Rudd replaced Julia Gillard as leader of the Labor Party, thus becoming Prime Minister, and announced plans to fast forward the ETS, owing to high living costs19. The ETS was linked to the European Unions ETS one year earlier than planned, creating a floating market-based price for carbon, instead of a fixed price. The CELP and associated ETS was later repealed in July 2014, after the conservative Liberal–National Party coalition won government in the September 2013 federal election, and Tony Abbott became the new Prime Minister. A new Direct Action Plan was enacted, in which an Emissions Reduction Fund provides incentives for abatement activities22.

We investigated whether the 2013 Australian federal election affected levels of acceptance and support for the Australian carbon policy. The carbon policy was a key election issue, with different carbon policy outcomes depending on the election results. We measured acceptance and support of the carbon policy two weeks before (Time 1—T1) and two weeks after (Time 2—T2) the election through an online questionnaire distributed to a national sample of Australian residents. We tested the following hypotheses: acceptance will be higher than support; acceptance and support will change between T1 and T2; and acceptance is a necessary precondition of support (as one must accept a policy to support it).

At both T1 and T2, policy acceptance was significantly higher than support (T1: t(515) = 9.68, p < 0.001; T2: t(515) = 11.64, p < 0.001) and, surprisingly, acceptance and support were unchanged by the election (repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA); acceptance: F(1,515) = 1.44, p > 0.10, support: F(1,515) = 0.01, p > 0.10; see Table 1). Although acceptance and support levels varied predictably with the political party voted for, the basic findings of higher levels of acceptance than support and lack of change over time were consistent across party choice (see Table 1 and Methods for complete analysis).

Table 1: Mean values with standard deviations of acceptance and support by party (first preference votes).

We used a within-subjects online survey experiment to investigate changes in support and acceptance before (T1) and after (T2) the 2013 federal election with Australian adults representing metropolitan, regional and rural areas online. Participants were recruited from a research-only panel administered by the research company ORU (http://www.theoru.com). ORU is an online fieldwork company with QSOAP Gold Standard and new global ISO 26363 standard accreditations. Through multi-source and multichannel recruiting, ORU has a database of more than 300,000 individuals from across Australia. Participants were entered into a prize draw as an incentive to complete the survey. T1 surveying occurred approximately two weeks before the election in August 2013, and T2 occurred approximately two weeks after the election. Only T1 participants willing to be contacted at T2 were retained for data analysis (N = 772, 384 males, 388 females). The T1 (T2) survey was open for six (seven) days before we reached the desired minimum sample size of 750.

In addition, we used between-subjects design to measure testing bias. Testing bias was assessed to ensure that participating in the survey before the election did not influence the responses of those same participants (within-subjects) after the election. Post-election results from an independent sample taken at T2 were compared against the within-subjects sample at T2 and no significant differences were found on measures of acceptance or support (p′s > 0.05), indicating there were no testing effects for our online survey.

For T1, we sent 14,500 invites via email within the panel. Of these, 1,551 respondents clicked the link from their computers, resulting in a 48.8% cooperation rate, and 5.3% response rate. For T2 we re-contacted the 772 T1 participants and recruited new participants. At T2, N = 785 (388 males, 397 females); 516 were T1 participants and 269 were new participants. There was a 72.6% cooperation rate; 66.8% response rate within the T1 sample (772 invites sent, 711 click-ins), and a 55% cooperation rate; 5.7% response rate for those who were new at T2 (4,700 invites sent, 489 click-ins).

Questionnaires differed slightly for T1 and T2, reflecting prior consent and the recent change in government. Our measures are described below (see Supplementary Information for questionnaire). Our method is, in part, based on past work1, but improves on certain limitations by utilizing the same response format to measure multiple items for acceptance and support. In addition, our analytical approach differs from that past work. Unless noted, items were measured on five-point response scales.

Free-market ideology (FMI).

We measured FMI using the FMI scale in ref. 18 (see Supplementary Information), presenting items in random order to each participant. Responses were coded so a higher score aligned with endorsement of a FMI.

Policy acceptance and support.

To ensure participants had a basic policy knowledge, the survey included a brief summary of the carbon policy taken from the government website. Next, acceptance and support items were randomly presented to each participant, along with the fairness and effectiveness items. The acronym CELP is used here for brevity, but was spelt out within questionnaire items.

We measured acceptance with four statements: ‘How acceptable do you find the CELP?, ‘To what extent are you in favour for or against the CELP?, ‘To what extent do you agree or disagree with the implementation of the CELP? and ‘Do you prefer having the current carbon policy in place, as opposed to no carbon policy?. Scores were averaged; higher numbers indicate greater acceptance (T1: α = 0.95; T2: α = 0.95).

Support was measured with three statements: ‘How supportive are you of the carbon policy (The CELP)?, ‘How willing are you to bear some of the costs resulting from the implementation of the CELP?, ‘How willing are you to take action to voice a positive opinion about the CELP, such as writing a letter or calling a representative?. Scores were averaged; higher numbers indicate greater support (T1: α = 0.79; T2: α = 0.82).

Perceived fairness and effectiveness.

Perceived fairness and effectiveness were each measured by two items: ‘How fair do you think it is that some big industries now must pay for the carbon they emit, as mandated by the carbon pricing policy?, How fair do you think the compensation plan is for those affected by increased costs due to the carbon price?, ‘How effective do you think the carbon pricing policy will be to help lower carbon emissions from industries in Australia?, ‘How effective do you think the compensation plan is in reducing the financial impact of the carbon price on individuals?. The two items for each measure were strongly correlated with each other (T1: reff = 0.65, rfair = 0.54; T2: reff = 0.65, rfair = 0.49; p′s < 0.01), supporting our decision to take the average of the two items as our measures of fairness and effectiveness. Higher numbers indicate greater fairness/effectiveness.

Voting preferences and behaviour.

At T1 we asked ‘Which political party are you most likely to vote for in the next federal election?; and at T2 ‘In the recent federal election for the House of Representatives, which of the following did you vote for?, and ‘To which party did you give your second preference?. Response categories were: Labor Party, Liberal Party, National Party, Greens Party, Independent, Family First, Other, Prefer not to say. Additionally, there was an ‘I dont know category at T1.

Demographics.

We assessed gender, income, age and education. Six response categories were included for income, varying from ‘0–$18,200 to ‘$180,001 and over. These categories were chosen to replicate the cutoff points for the compensation package within the carbon policy, based on household incomes established by the Australian Government. Additionally, we included a ‘prefer not to respond category.

Acceptance and support.

During both time periods, policy acceptance was higher than support. Of the 516 participants who completed both surveys, 46.3% found the policy acceptable, 28.1% unacceptable, and 25.6% were neutral or ambivalent at T1. In contrast, 35.9% supported the policy, 33.5% did not support the policy, and 30.6% were neutral or ambivalent. At T2, 45.7% found the policy acceptable, 25.4% unacceptable, and 28.9% were neutral or ambivalent. In contrast, 36.4% supported the policy, 33.3% did not support the policy, and 30.2% were neutral or ambivalent. These findings were further supported by significant mean differences between acceptance and support for both time periods (Table 1). As hypothesized, participants showed greater acceptance than support of the policy at T1 (t(515) = 9.68, p < 0.001) and T2 (t(515) = 11.64, p < 0.001). Both acceptance and support were stable across the election time: a repeated-measures ANOVA showed no significant change in acceptance or support from T1 to T2, (p′s > 0.05).

Covariates.

Preliminary regression analyses for covariates revealed that age and gender were not significantly related to support or acceptance—and were therefore dropped from subsequent analyses. Education and income were significant predictors of acceptance (b = 0.14, p < 0.01; b = −0.11, p < 0.05) and education was a significant predictor of support (b = 0.19, p < 0.01). As the inclusion of these variables did not affect the significance of other variables within the hierarchical multiple regression models, they were removed from the models for sake of parsimony and comparability. In addition, education and income were analysed for repeated-measures effects, to see if the covariates influenced changes in acceptance or support over time. No covariate significantly impacted the relationship of acceptance or support over time. Therefore, the covariates were excluded from analysis of variance procedures.

Analysis of variance by voting.

Political party voting intention at T1 was closely split between the Labor Party (n = 148, 28.7%) and the Liberal Party (n = 146, 28.3%), with the remainder spread among smaller parties. At T2, self-reported first preference votes (the political party to which the participants gave their first vote to) were equally split between the Labor Party (n = 164) and the Liberal Party (n = 165), with other parties in smaller quantities (Table 1).

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to see whether supporters of different political parties significantly differed in carbon policy acceptance and support. This analysis included only Labor, Liberal and Greens Party supporters owing to small cell sizes of other categories and lack of comparability for ‘prefer not to say or ‘other. The analyses revealed significant main effects for voting behaviour on acceptance and support and both T1 and T2 (Acceptance: F(2,373) = 79.99, p < 0.001; F(2,373) = 73.46, p < 0.001, respectively; Support: F(2,373) = 66.81, p < 0.001; F(2,373) = 57.68, p < 0.001, respectively). Tukeys HSD tests showed significant differences between Greens and Liberal supporters, and Liberal and Labor supporters at T1 and T2, as well as Greens and Labor at T2, but only for support (p′s < 0.05) (Table 1). Highest levels of acceptance and support of the carbon policy occurred in Greens Party supporters at both T1 and T2, whereas Liberal Party supporters reported the lowest levels of acceptance and support. Paired samples t-tests indicated no changes in acceptance or support from T1 to T2 for Labor, Liberal or Greens supporters (p′s < 0.05).

Cross-lagged correlations.

Owing to high zero-order correlation between the acceptance and support at both time periods, we computed cross-lagged correlations on the residualsURL:

http://www.nature.com/nclimate/journal/v5/n12/full/nclimate2756.html
Citation statistics:
资源类型: 期刊论文
标识符: http://119.78.100.158/handle/2HF3EXSE/4626
Appears in Collections:气候变化事实与影响
科学计划与规划
气候变化与战略

Files in This Item: Download All
File Name/ File Size Content Type Version Access License
nclimate2756.pdf(410KB)期刊论文作者接受稿开放获取View Download

Recommended Citation:
Stacia J. Dreyer. Australians' views on carbon pricing before and after the 2013 federal election[J]. Nature Climate Change,2015-08-10,Volume:5:Pages:1064;1067 (2015).
Service
Recommend this item
Sava as my favorate item
Show this item's statistics
Export Endnote File
Google Scholar
Similar articles in Google Scholar
[Stacia J. Dreyer]'s Articles
百度学术
Similar articles in Baidu Scholar
[Stacia J. Dreyer]'s Articles
CSDL cross search
Similar articles in CSDL Cross Search
[Stacia J. Dreyer]‘s Articles
Related Copyright Policies
Null
收藏/分享
文件名: nclimate2756.pdf
格式: Adobe PDF
此文件暂不支持浏览
所有评论 (0)
暂无评论
 

Items in IR are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.